Where is statistics used inappropriately




















There are many ways to manipulate data, including but not limited to inappropriate use of descriptive statistics. Knowing about them will help you spot them. Following are some misleading stats examples. This kind of data display can confuse and show the difference bigger than it is. You can identify misleading graphs in the media if you look at the numbers and see how much variation is in the numbers. For example, the Fox News poll compared people on welfare and the number of people with jobs is a prime example of selective data display.

It included all people on welfare from a house in which one or more people were on welfare regardless of other members of the family who were not on welfare. To the contrary, only a person on full-time job was included in on job sample. The graph is not only an example of bad sampling but also exaggerated the difference.

If you look closely, the scale of the graph starts at million instead of zero. The data is presented in a way that shows the number of people on welfare four times more than the number of people with jobs. Another way you can identify a misleading graph is to check if the start-at-zero-rule has been followed. Y-Axis manipulation is also often used to change the perception of the real data.

This graph shows the temperatures from degrees to degrees. The flat line gives the idea that global warming is not a problem. While investigating, The school's statistician Peter Bickel looked at the data and found that only 2 out of 4 departments showed the statistically significant gender bias but in favor of women. The women applied to the departments that admitted a smaller percentage of applicants overall-this was the hidden variable that reversed the trend in the data as a whole.

Wherein the average suggested that the male applicants were admitted at a higher rate than females. But the data showed a different story when it was divided into departments.

Often people confuse correlation with causation, but the correlation between the two variables does not imply causation. Therefore, make sure to understand the difference and look for a cause and effect relationship. In reality, this is a famous example of misleading statistics.

The adsuggested that dentists preferred Colgate over other toothpaste brands. But the survey asked them to list several brands of toothpaste they would recommend. The data only showed that Colgate was one of a number of different brands that dentists liked. Not quite the same claim, was it? Businesses use information from surveys and other sources every day to make critical business decisions.

But sometimes data can be deceiving. There are three main stages in the data analysis process where issues can occur:. In the Colgate example above, the problem was at the presentation stage. The company left out critical information when they revealed the results of their study. Misleading statistics pose a serious concern with internal operations, not just external promotions. Collecting data from too small a group can skew your survey and test results.

Small samples underrepresent your target audience. They can lead to misleading statistics that give you a faulty idea of customer satisfaction and product preferences. Sample size is especially important if you analyze results in terms of percentages instead of exact numbers. If you tested 10, people, that percentage is a pretty convincing reason to develop that version. But if you tested only 20 people, that means only 12 are interested in the idea.

Twelve is too small a group to show you that the new feature will be worth your investment. Small group sizes can also lead to biased sampling. The smaller the group, the less likely it is to represent the diverse demographics of your customer base. An ideal sample size depends on many factors, like your company and the goals for your project.

Using a third-party tool helps you reliably assess your sample size without having to figure out the calculations on your own. Users enter their expected conversion rate and the percent change they are looking for.

The way you word survey questions can also be a source of misleading statistics. A recent UK study shows that the way you phrase a question directly affects how a person answers. One example is survey questions that ask for confirmation. Essentially, you are including the answer in the question.

Check your surveys for manipulative wording that might lead respondents to give a particular answer. A few examples of influential phrasing include:. Check for leading language by asking co-workers to review surveys before sending to customers.

Ask what parts of your questions, if any, suggest how they should respond. Confirmation bias is when you have a set result you want or expect to see, so you look at only data that affirms your belief. Actually, there is no problem per se — but there can be. Statistics are infamous for their ability and potential to exist as misleading and bad data.

Misleading statistics are simply the misusage - purposeful or not - of a numerical data. The results provide a misleading information to the receiver, who then believes something wrong if he or she does not notice the error or the does not have the full data picture. As an exercise in due diligence, we will review some of the most common forms of misuse of statistics, and various alarming and sadly, common misleading statistics examples from public life.

Statistical reliability is crucial in order to ensure the precision and validity of the analysis. To make sure the reliability is high, there are various techniques to perform — first of them being the control tests, that should have similar results when reproducing an experiment in similar conditions.

However, the telling of half-truths through study is not only limited to mathematical amateurs. A investigative survey by Dr.

Daniele Fanelli from The University of Edinburgh found that There are different ways how statistics can be misleading that we will detail later. The most common one is of course correlation versus causation, that always leaves out another or two or three factor that are the actual causation of the problem.

Did we forget to mention the amount of sugar put in the tea, or the fact that baldness and old age are related — just like cardiovascular disease risks and old age? So, can statistics be manipulated? They sure can. Do numbers lie? You can be the judge. Remember, misuse of statistics can be accidental or purposeful.

While a malicious intent to blur lines with misleading statistics will surely magnify bias, intent is not necessary to create misunderstandings. The misuse of statistics is a much broader problem that now permeates through multiple industries and fields of study.

Here are a few potential mishaps that commonly lead to misuse:. The manner in which questions are phrased can have a huge impact on the way an audience answers them. Specific wording patterns have a persuasive effect and induce respondents to answer in a predictable manner.

These two questions are likely to provoke far different responses, even though they deal with the same topic of government assistance. The latter two examples of the original questions eliminate any inference or suggestion from the poller, and thus, are significantly more impartial. Another unfair method of polling is to ask a question, but precede it with a conditional statement or a statement of fact.

A good rule of thumb is to always take polling with a grain of salt, and to try to review the questions that were actually presented. They provide great insight, often more so than the answers. The problem with correlations is this: if you measure enough variables, eventually it will appear that some of them correlate. As one out of twenty will inevitably be deemed significant without any direct correlation, studies can be manipulated with enough data to prove a correlation that does not exist or that is not significant enough to prove causation.

Any sensible person would easily identify the fact that car accidents do not cause bear attacks. Each is likely a result of a third factor, that being: an increased population, due to high tourism season in the month of June. It would be preposterous to say that they cause each other It is easy to see a correlation.

But, what about causation? What if the measured variables were different? Clearly there is a correlation between the two, but is there causation? Many would falsely assume, yes, solely based on the strength of the correlation.

Tread carefully, for either knowingly or ignorantly, correlation hunting will continue to exist within statistical studies.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000